Salta al menu principale di navigazione Salta al contenuto principale Salta al piè di pagina del sito

Articoli

N. 55 (2022)

Una casa di 3 piani + 1. Il sogno di Jung e le omologie archetipiche cervello-mente in una prospettiva evolutiva

DOI
https://doi.org/10.3280/jun55-2022oa14057
Inviata
5 July 2022
Pubblicato
02-08-2022

Abstract

Sulla nave che lo portava verso le Clark lectures negli Stati Uniti, Jung racconta a Freud un sogno, divenuto famoso, in cui una casa a quattro piani, o livelli, sembra rappresentare la struttura di una psiche fondata sull’inconscio archetipico. Nonostante l’idea di archetipo sia stata più volte oggetto di critiche, a partire dalla seconda metà del 1900 gli studi scientifici sull’organizzazione del cervello umano hanno confermato l’ipotesi di una stratificazione delle funzioni mentali e di una determinazione prevalentemente istintuale ed ereditaria del primo e più antico strato dell’evoluzione neuropsichica. Pertanto, riprendendo la struttura della casa sognata da Jung, in questo articolo proponiamo l’idea di una stratificazione psico-neuro-archeologica suddivisa in 3+1 strati sovrapposti che costituisce una elaborazione del modello neuro-archeologico ternario elaborato da Paul MacLean prima e da Jaak Panksepp poi, in cui l’affettività rappresenta il fattore organizzativo fondamentale del cervello-mente. Lo strato più evoluto, caratteristico della specie umana, è quello dell’auto-coscienza riflessiva. Subito sotto si trova il livello della coscienza intersoggettiva, caratteristico delle specie omeoterme (mammiferi ed uccelli) e legato all’evoluzione di un complesso di strutture corticali mediali chiamate Default-Mode-Network. Ancora sotto si trova lo strato della coscienza cognitivo-immaginativa, evolutasi nei vertebrati dotati di corteccia cerebrale. Infine, il primo e più antico strato, è quello della consapevolezza affettiva, legato al funzionamento delle strutture sottocorticali mediali (core-Self), dove risiedono i circuiti istintuali ed archetipici individuati negli studi neuro-etologici di Panksepp.

Riferimenti bibliografici

  1. Alcaro A. (2019). Il soffio della consapevolezza ed il taglio della coscienza. Rivista di Psicologia Analitica, 48: 119-137.
  2. Alcaro A., Carta S. (2019). The “Instinct” of Imagination. A Neuro-Ethological Approach to the Evolution of the Reflective Mind and Its Application to Psychotherapy. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12: 522. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00522.
  3. Alcaro A., Carta S., Panksepp J. (2017). The affective core of the self: a neuro-archetypical perspective on the foundations of human (and animal) subjectivity. Front. Psychol., 8: 1424. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01424.
  4. Allen C., Trestman M. (2016). Animal consciousness. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford: Zalta E.N. (Winter Edition).
  5. Balduzzi D., Tononi G. (2009). Qualia: the geometry of integrated information. PLoS Comput Biol., 5,8: e1000462. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000462.
  6. Barkow J., Cosmides L., Tooby J. (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.
  7. Bergson H. (2002). The Creative Mind: An Introduction to Metaphysics. New York: Citadel Press.
  8. Bergson H. (1998). Matter and Memory. New York: Zone Books.
  9. Bernard C. (1961). An Introduction to the Study of Experimental Medicine. New York: Collier Books.
  10. Bion W. R. (1962). Learning from Experience. New York: Basic Books (trad. it. Apprendere dall’esperienza. Roma: Armando, 1972).
  11. Bohm D. (1980). Wholeness and the Implicate Order. Great Britain: Routledge.
  12. Brown D.E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  13. Brown J.W. (2002). The Self-Embodying Mind. Process, Brain Dynamics and the Conscious Present. New York: Barrytown.
  14. Calabretta R. (2001). Natural Selection and the origin of modules. In: Callebaut W. e Rasskin-Gutman D., a cura di, Modularity. Understanding the development and evolution of complex natural systems. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  15. Calabretta R. (2002). Connessionismo evolutivo e origine della modularità. In: Borghi A. e Iachini T., a cura di, Scienze della Mente. Bologna: il Mulino.
  16. Cosmides L., Tooby J. (1994). Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In: Hirschfeld L.A. and Gelman S.A., editors, Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  17. Dehaene S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York: Viking Penguin.
  18. Denton D.A. (2006). The Primordial Emotions: The Dawning of Consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.
  19. Edelman G.M., Tononi G. (2000). Un universo di coscienza. Come la materia diventa immaginazione. Milano: Einaudi.
  20. Edelman D.B., Baars B.J., Seth A.K. (2005). Identifying hallmarks of consciousness in nonmammalian species. Conscious Cogn., 14: 169-87. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.09.001.
  21. Edelman G.M. (1989). The Remembered Present: A Biological Theory of Consciousness. New York: Basic Books.
  22. Edelman G.M. (1992). Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind. New York: Basic Books.
  23. Fabbro F., Aglioti S.M., Bergamasco M., Clarici A. and Panksepp J. (2015). Evolutionary aspects of self-and world consciousness in vertebrates. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26, 9: 157. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00157.
  24. Fodor J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  25. Freeman W.J. (1999). How Brains Make-up Their Minds. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.
  26. Freud S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. In: Strachey J., editor, The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  27. Gallagher S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 1: 14-21.
  28. Griffin D. (1985). Animal Thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  29. Griffin D.R. (1981). The Question of Animal Awareness: evolutionary continuity of mental experience (revised and expanded edition). Los Altos, CA: William Kaufmann.
  30. Johnson M.H., Karmiloff-Smith A., Parisi D. and Plunkett K. (1996), Rethinking innateness. A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  31. Jouvet M. (1975). The function of dreaming: a neurophysiologist’s point of view. In: Gazzaniga M.S. and Blakemore C., editors, Handbook of Psychobiology. New York: Academic Press. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-278656-3.X5001-7.
  32. Jung C.G. (1937-1942). Psychological factors in human behavior. In: The Collected Works, Vol. 8. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  33. Jung C.G. (1947-1954). Theoretische Überlegungen zum Wesen des Psychischen (trad. it.: Riflessioni teoriche sull’essenza della psiche. In: Opere, vol. 8. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri, 1976).
  34. Jung C.G. (1955). Synchronicity: an acausal connecting principle. In: The Collected Works, 2nd Edn, Vol. 8. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  35. Karmiloff-Smith A. (1995). Beyond Modularity - A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  36. Kihlstrom J.F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237: 1445-52. DOI: 10.1126/science.3629249.
  37. Knox J. (2004). From archetypes to reflective function. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 49: 1-19 DOI: 10.1111/j.0021-8774.2004.0437.x.
  38. Krieger N.M. (2014). Bridges to Consciousness. Complexes and Complexity. New York: Routledge.
  39. Kuhn T.S. (1970). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
  40. Lakatos I. (1970). Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes. In: Lakatos I. and Musgrave A., editors, Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  41. Lakoff G., Núñez R. (2000). Where Mathematics Comes From. New York: Basic Books.
  42. Llinas R. (2002). I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  43. Lorenz K.Z. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels; Der Artgenosse als auslösendes Moment sozialer Verhaltungsweisen. Journal für Ornithologie, 83: pt. 2-3 (trad. engl.: The Companion in the Bird’s World; the fellow-member of the species as a releasing factor of social behavior. The Auk, 54,3, 1937).
  44. Mandler J.M. (2004). The Foundations of Mind: Origins of Conceptual Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  45. Marr D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Walter Freeman and Co.
  46. McFadden J. (2013). The CEMI field theory: Closing the loop. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 20, 1, 2: 153-168.
  47. MacLean P.D. (1982). On the Origin and Progressive Evolution of the Triune Brain. In: Armstrong E. and Falk D., editors, Primate Brain Evolution. Boston, MA: Springer.
  48. Minelli A. (2007). Forme del divenire. Evo-devo: la biologia evoluzionistica dello sviluppo. Milano: Einaudi.
  49. Nagarjuna G. (2005a, July). From folklore to science. Paper presented at the International History, Philosophy, Sociology & Science Teaching Conference, Leeds.
  50. Narayanan S. (1997). KARMA: knowledge-based active representations for metaphor and aspect. PhD dissertation, Computer Science Division. Berkeley, USA: University of California https://www1.icsi.berkeley.edu/~snarayan/thesis.pdf.
  51. Nunn C. (2016). New Directions in Consciousness Studies: SoS Theory and the Nature of Time. London: Routledge.
  52. Panksepp J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
  53. Panksepp J., Biven L. (2012). The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotion. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
  54. Pauli W. (2006). Psiche e Natura. Milano: Adelphi.
  55. Pavlov I.P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex (translated and edited by Anrep G.V.). London: Oxford University Press.
  56. Penrose R. (1989). The Emperor’s New Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  57. Pereira Jr. A., Furlan F.A. (2010). Astrocytes and Human Cognition: modeling information integration and modulation of neuronal activity. Progress in Neurobiology, 92,3: 405-20. DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.07.001.
  58. Pereira Jr. A. (2017). Astroglial hydro-ionic waves guided by the extracellular matrix: an exploratory model. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 16: 1-16. DOI: 10.3233/JIN-160003.
  59. Pinker S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.
  60. Reiter S., Liaw H.P., Yamawaki T.M., Naumann R.K. and Lauren G. (2017). On the value of reptilian brains to map the evolution of the hippocampal formation. Brain Behav. Evol, 90: 41-52. DOI: 10.1159/000478693.
  61. Rumelhart D., McClelland J. and the PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  62. Sander L. (2008). Living Systems, Evolving Consciousness, and the Emerging Person A Selection of Papers from the Life Work of Louis Sander (Amadei G. e Bianchi I., a cura di). London: Routledge.
  63. Seth A., Suzuki K. and Critchley H. (2012). An Interoceptive Predictive Coding Model of Conscious Presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2: 395. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395.
  64. Shein-Idelson M., Ondracek J.M., Liaw H.P., Reitere S. and Laurent G. (2016). Slow waves, sharp waves, ripples, and REM in sleeping dragons. Science, 352: 590-95. DOI: 10.1126/science.aaf3621.
  65. Skinner B.F. (1935). Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudo-type. Journal of General Psychology, 12: 66-77. DOI: 10.1080/00221309.1935.9920088.
  66. Sloman A. (2020). Biological evolution’s use of representational redescription. In: Taking Development Seriously: A festschrifts for Annette Karmiloff-Smith. London: Routledge.
  67. Solms K., Solms M. (2000). Clinical Studies in Neuro-Psychoanalysis. London: Karnac Books (trad. it.: Neuropsicoanalisi. Un’introduzione clinica alla neuropsicologia del profondo. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 2002).
  68. Solms M., Panksepp, J. (2012). The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sci, 2: 147-175. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci2020147.
  69. Stern D. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books (trad. it.: Il mondo interpersonale del bambino. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri, 1987).
  70. Tinbergen N. (1951). The study of instinct. London and New York: Oxford University Press (trad. it.: Lo studio dell’istinto. Milano: Adelphi, 1994).
  71. Tulving E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology., 26: 1-12. DOI: 10.1037/h0080017.
  72. Vandekerckhove M., Panksepp, J. (2009). The flow of anoetic to noetic and autonoetic consciousness: a vision of unknowing (anoetic) and knowing (noetic) consciousness in the remembrance of things past and imagined futures. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 18, 4: 1018-1028. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.002.
  73. Vandekerckhove M., Panksepp J. (2011). A neurocognitive theory of higher mental emergence: from anoetic affective experiences to noetic knowledge and autonoetic awareness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev., 35, 9: 2017‐2025. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.001.
  74. Weir A.A.S., Chappel J., Kacelnik A. (2002). Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science, 297: 981. DOI: 10.1126/science.1073433.

Metriche

Caricamento metriche ...