Salta al menu principale di navigazione Salta al contenuto principale Salta al piè di pagina del sito

Articoli

N. 1 (2023)

La pandemia da Covid-19 e i ricordi autobiografici emozionali del primo lockdown

DOI
https://doi.org/10.3280/rip2023oa16769
Inviata
10 novembre 2023
Pubblicato
24-11-2023

Abstract

Il presente studio si proponeva di indagare gli effetti del primo lockdown della pandemia da Covid-19 sulla memoria autobiografica emozionale di un gruppo di adulti della provincia di Bergamo, uno dei territori che ha sperimentato le conseguenze più drammatiche della prima ondata della pandemia da Covid-19. Hanno partecipato allo studio 83 persone (43 femmine e 40 maschi) di età compresa tra i 20 e i 69 anni. La ricerca è stata condotta sul web mediante la somministrazione di tre strumenti self-report: un compito di narrazione autobiografica tramite il quale è stato chiesto ai partecipanti di raccontare un evento emozionale che avevano vissuto durante il primo lockdown, un questionario sulle caratteristiche fenomenologiche del ricordo e una scala per la valutazione della centralità dell’evento per la storia di vita dei partecipanti. Dall’analisi del contenuto delle narrazioni autobiografiche è emerso che l’86% dei partecipanti aveva raccontato un evento emozionale con valenza edonica negativa, mentre il restante 14% aveva raccontato un evento con valenza positiva. Gli eventi negativi più citati includevano decessi e ricoveri ospedalieri dovuti al Covid; quelli positivi avevano a che fare invece con attività di svago, riunioni familiari e guarigioni dal Covid. Ricordi negativi e ricordi positivi presentavano delle somiglianze sostanziali in relazione alle loro caratteristiche narrative e fenomenologiche e al loro grado di centralità. Nelle conclusioni, gli autori interpretano i risultati alla luce della letteratura contemporanea sulla memoria autobiografica emozionale e delle caratteristiche del contesto di emergenza indagato.

Riferimenti bibliografici

  1. Aknin, L. B., De Neve, J. E., Dunn, E. W., Fancourt, D. E., Goldberg, E., Helliwell, J. F., Jones, S. P., Karam, E., Layard, R., Lyubomirsky, S., Rzepa, A., Saxena, S., Thornton, E. M., VanderWeele, T. J., Whillans, A. V., Zaki, J., Karadag, O., & Ben Amor, Y. (2022). Mental health during the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic: A review and recommendations for moving forward. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(4), 915-936. DOI: 10.1177/17456916211029964.
  2. American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (v ed., text revision). American Psychiatric Association.
  3. Arora, T., Grey, I., Östlundh, L., Lam, K. B. H., Omar, O. M., & Arnone D. (2022). The prevalence of psychological consequences of COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Journal of Health Psychology, 27(4), 805-824. DOI: 10.1177/1359105320966639.
  4. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2006). The Centrality of Event Scale: A measure of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 219-231. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.01.009.
  5. Boals, A. (2010). Events that have become central to identity: Gender differences in the centrality of events scale for positive and negative events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 107-121. DOI: 10.1002/acp.1548.
  6. Bohanek, J. G., Fivush, R., & Walker, E. (2005). Memories of positive and negative emotional events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 51-66. DOI: 10.1002/acp.1064.
  7. Brown, N. R. (2021). The possible effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the contents and organization of autobiographical memory: A Transition-Theory perspective. Cognition, 212. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104694.
  8. Butler, L. D., & Wolfner, A. L. (2000). Some characteristics of positive and negative (“most traumatic”) event memories in a college sample. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 1, 45-68.
  9. Byrne, C. A., Hyman, I. E., & Scott, K. L. (2001). Comparisons of memories for traumatic events and other experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 119-133. DOI: 10.1002/acp.837.
  10. Enander, A., & Wallenius, C. (1999). Psychological reactions and experiences among Swedish citizens resident in Kobe during the 1995 earthquake. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 17(2), 185-205. DOI: 10.1177/028072709901700203.
  11. Er, N. (2003). A new flashbulb memory model applied to the Marmara earthquake. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 503-517. DOI: 10.1002/acp.870.
  12. Habermas, T. (2019). Emotion and narrative: Perspectives in autobiographical storytelling. Cambridge University Press.
  13. Istat & ISS (2020). Impatto dell’epidemia Covid-19 sulla mortalità totale della popolazione residente. Primo trimestre 2020. Documento disponibile al sito internet: https://www.istat.it/it/files/2020/05/Rapporto_Istat_ISS.pdf.
  14. Kraft, R. N. (2002). Memory perceived: Recalling the Holocaust. Praeger.
  15. Kunzler, A. M., Röthke, N., Günthner, L., Stoffers-Winterling, J., Tüscher, O., Coenen, M., Rehfuess, E., Schwarzer, G., Binder, H., Schmucker, C., Meerpohl, J. J., & Lieb, K. (2021). Mental burden and its risk and protective factors during the early phase of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: Systematic review and meta-analyses. Globalization and Health, 17(34). DOI: 10.1186/s12992-021-00670-y.
  16. Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22(5), 530-554. DOI: 10.1521/soco.22.5.530.50767.
  17. Lopez-Persem, A., Bieth, T., Guiet, S, Ovando-Tellez, M., & Volle, E. (2022). Through thick and thin: Changes in creativity during the first lockdown of the COVID-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 821550. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.821550.
  18. Luo, M., Guo, L., Yu, M., Jiang, W., & Wang, H. (2020). The psychological and mental impact of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) on medical staff and general public - A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 291, 113190. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113190.
  19. Morse, K. F., Fine, P. A., & Friedlander, K. J. (2021). Creativity and leisure during COVID-19: Examining the relationship between leisure activities, motivations, and psychological well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 609967. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609967.
  20. Neisser, U., Winograd, E., Bergman, E. T., Schreiber, C. A., Palmer, S. E., & Weldon, M. S. (1996). Remembering the earthquake: Direct experience vs. hearing the news. Memory, 4(4), 337-357.DOI: 10.1080/096582196388898.
  21. Paradis, C. M., Solomon, L. Z., Florer, F., & Thompson, T. (2004). Flashbulb memories of personal events of 9/11 and the day after for a sample of New York City residents. Psychological Reports, 95(1), 304-310. DOI: 10.2466/PR0.95.5.304-310.
  22. Peace, K. A., & Porter, S. (2004). A longitudinal investigation of the reliability of memories for trauma and other emotional experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1143-1159. DOI: 10.1002/acp.1046.
  23. Pezdek, K. (2003). Event memory and autobiographical memory for the events of September 11, 2001. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(9), 1033-1045. DOI: 10.1002/acp.984.
  24. Porter, S., & Birt, A. R. (2001). Is traumatic memory special? A comparison of traumatic memory characteristics with memory for other emotional life experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 101-117. DOI: 10.1002/acp.766.
  25. Rajkumar, R. (2020). COVID-19 and mental health: A review of the existing literature. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 52, 102066. DOI: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102066.
  26. Reese, E., Haden, C. A., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P., Fivush, R., & Ornstein, P. A. (2011). Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: a multidimensional model and coding method. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(4), 424-462. DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2011.587854.
  27. Salari, N., Hosseinian-Far, A., Jalali, R., Vaisi-Raygani, A., Rasoulpoor, S., Mohammadi, M., Rasoulpoor, S., & Khaledi-Paveh, B. (2020). Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Globalization and Health, 16(57). DOI: 10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w.
  28. Schaefer, A., & Philippot, P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory, 13(2), 148-160. doi: 10.1080/09658210344000648.
  29. Schelach, L., & Nachson, I. (2001). Memory of Auschwitz survivors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 119-132. DOI: 10.1002/1099-
  30. (200103/04)15:2<119::AID-ACP692>3.0.CO;2-N.
  31. Shen, X., MacDonald, M., Logan, S. W., Parkinson, C., Gorrell, L., & Hatfield, B. E. (2022). Leisure engagement during COVID-19 and its association with mental health and wellbeing in U.S. Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(3), 1081. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph19031081.
  32. Skowronski, J. J., Walker, W. R., Henderson, D. X., & Bond, G. D. (2014). The fading affect bias: Its history, its implications, and its future. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 49, pp. 163-218). Academic Press.
  33. Sotgiu, I. (2016). How do we remember happy life events? A comparison between eudaimonic and hedonic autobiographical memories. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 150(6), 685-703. DOI: 10.1080/00223980.2016.1162764.
  34. Sotgiu, I. (2019). Gender differences and similarities in autobiographical memory for eudaimonic happy events. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20, 1457-1479. DOI: 10.1007/s10902-018-0006-z.
  35. Sotgiu, I. (2021). The psychology of autobiographical memory: History, theory, research. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-69571-2.
  36. Sotgiu, I., & Galati, D. (2007). Long-term memory for traumatic events: Experiences and emotional reactions during the 2000 flood in Italy. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 141(1), 91-108. DOI: 10.3200/JRLP.141.1.91-108.
  37. Sotgiu, I., & Mormont, C. (2008). Similarities and differences between traumatic and emotional memories: Review and directions for future research. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 142(5), 449-469. DOI: 10.3200/JRLP.142.5.449-470.
  38. Sotgiu, I., & Rusconi, M. L. (2014). Why autobiographical memories for traumatic and emotional events might differ: Theoretical arguments and empirical evidence. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 148(5), 523-547. DOI: 10.1080/00223980.2013.814619.
  39. Torales, J., O’Higgins, M., Castaldelli-Maia, J. M., & Ventriglio, A. (2020). The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 66(4), 317-320. DOI: 10.1177/0020764020915212.
  40. Vindegaard, N., & Benros, M. E. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic and mental health consequences: Systematic review of the current evidence. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 89, 531-542. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.048.
  41. Wagenaar, W. A., & Groeneweg, J. (1990). The memory of concentration camp survivors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4(2), 77-87. DOI: 10.1002/acp.2350040202.
  42. Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003). Life is pleasant And memory helps to keep it that way! Review of General Psychology, 7(2), 203-210. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203.
  43. Waters, T. E., Bohanek, J. G., Marin, K., & Fivush, R. (2013). Null’s the word: A comparison of memory quality for intensely negative and positive events. Memory, 21, 633-645. DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2012.745877.

Metriche

Caricamento metriche ...